ES EN
Vol. 22. Núm. 1. - 2016. Páginas 19-26

Cyberbullying in youth: A pattern of disruptive behaviour

[El ciberacoso en la juventud: Un patrón de comportamiento disruptivo]

Anna C. Baldry1 , David P. Farrington2 , Anna Sorrentino1
1Second Univ. Naples, Dep. Psychology, Italy ,2Univ. Cambridge, United Kingdom


https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pse.2016.02.001

Abstract

Despite the growing diffusion of cyberbullying among students and the numerous studies in the literature, to date relatively little is known about its relationship with school bullying. This article seeks to understand if there is an overlap between the roles of bullies and victims in traditional and electronic bullying. In order to investigate this, 5,058 Italian middle and high school students were surveyed about their experiences of cyberbullying and cybervictimisation, looking also at their involvement in school bullying. The results highlighted a significant overlap between school bullying and cyberbullying, with 12.1% of all students who bullied others at least sometimes being also cyberbullies. Similarly, there was a significant overlap between school victimisation and cybervictimisation, with 7.4% of all students who were victimised at school at least sometimes being also cybervictimised. Our findings confirm the existence of an overlap between school bullying and cyberbullying. We discuss useful intervention programs to reduce or prevent cyberbullying.

Resumen

A pesar de que aumenta la extensión del ciberacoso en estudiantes y de los muchos estudios al respecto, se sabe relativamente poco hasta el momento acerca de su relación con el acoso escolar. El artículo trata de entender si hay solapamiento entre los roles de agresor y víctima en el acoso tradicional y electrónico. Con el objeto de investigarlo se encuestó a 5,058 estudiantes italianos de secundaria y bachillerato sobre su experiencia de ciberacoso y cibervictimización, analizando igualmente la implicación en el acoso escolar. Los resultados ponen de manifiesto un solapamiento importante entre el acoso escolar y el ciberacoso: un 12.1% de todos los estudiantes que habían acosado a los demás al menos algunas veces habían sido también ciberacosadores. También había un solapamiento importante entre la victimización escolar y la cibervictimización: 7.4% de los estudiantes victimizados en la escuela al menos algunas veces habían sido también cibervictimizados. Los resultados confirman la existencia de solapamiento entre acoso escolar y ciberacoso. Se comentan programas de intervención útiles para disminuir o evitar el ciberacoso. 

In the last decade, publications and public discourses on bullying online, also called cyberbullying, have increased in number ( Ybarra & Mitchell, 2004 ). Still, however, a scientific debate is going on with regard to the relationship between school bullying and cyberbullying and whether or not cyberbullying is an alarming problem ( Olweus, 2012 ). Some of the questions addressed in the literature refer to whether cyberbullying is affecting the same students as traditional school bullying, whether cyberbullying is affecting boys and girls in the same way as traditional school bullying, and what types of online antisocial behaviours mainly take place.

The aim of the present study is to provide prevalence data on cyberbullying in its different forms in a representative sample of Italian students to study gender differences and to investigate the overlap between school bullying and cyberbullying and school victimisation and cybervictimisation in a large sample of adolescents and preadolescents in Italy.

Cyberbullying can be defined as ‘an aggressive act or behaviour that is carried out using electronic means by a group or an individual repeatedly and over time against a victim who cannot easily defend him or herself’ ( Smith et al., 2008 , p. 376). Other definitions subsequently better defined cyberbullying as including the intention of harming ( Slonje & Smith, 2008) and imbalance of power (Tokunaga, 2010). Patchin and Hinduja (2006, 2015) d also repetition in time and extended the means a cyberbully can use: computers, mobile phones, and other electronic devices. With regard to repetition in time, there is a debate in the literature as to whether cyberbullying, given its potential of going viral and reaching an infinite potential number of people even with only one single act, needs to be repeated in time by the cyberbully or not to be defined as such. Traditionally, the definition of school bullying s repeated acts.

Even if most researchers agree that cyberbullying can be considered as a new type of aggression, made possible by the increasing diffusion of the internet and the new information and communication technologies (ICTs) among young people ( Slonje, Smith, & Frisén, 2013 ), assessing the prevalence and nature of cyberbullying could be complex, since there is still a lack of consensus regarding how cyberbullying should be defined and measured ( Kowalski, Giumetti, Schroeder, & Lattanner, 2014; Olweus, 2013; Smith, del Barrio, & Tokunaga, 2013; Tokunaga, 2010; Ybarra, Boyd, Korchmaros, & Oppenheim, 2012 ). Patchin and Hinduja (2015) outline how different measures of cyberbullying do not take into account all components of the definition: intention of harm, imbalance of power, and repetition in time. Also, when reporting results, different authors use different criteria to classify students as belonging to one or another category. Some use a 4-level category (only cyberbullies, only cybervictims, both cyberbully and cybervictim, and not involved), while others use a dichotomous criterion (yes/no bully or victim) regardless of the other category. Therefore, classifying a student as a cyberbully (or cybervictim) or not is not easy.

There are also differences in how to allocate a student in one or another category (cyberbullying once or twice, at least three times) and what reference period should be used (in a 2 or 6 month-period prior to data collection, ever in the life course, in the last thirty days, or in the last term). This makes it difficult to make comparisons between studies and to have a shared agreement on how often cyberbullying takes place ( Del Rey et al., 2015; Patchin & Hinduja, 2015 ). However, by bearing these limitations in mind, we can look at what the studies have shown so far and present our study by providing prevalence data that try to explain the nature and prevalence of cyberbullying in a large Italian student sample.

Whatever the definition and methods used to study cyberbullying, it is clear that cyberbullying is a problem among young people. Year after year, due to the increasing access to technology, cheap internet contract or free wireless and the almost worldwide presence of internet, the risk of cyberbullying increases and the assessment of such risks is more and more needed ( Baldry, Farrington, & Sorrentino, 2015 ).

Wolak, Mitchell, and Finkelhor (2007) found that, between 2000 and 2005, rates of internet harassment increased by about 50%; this might imply that the more these means are available to young people, and at a younger age, the more the rates go up ( Willard, 2007 ). This increase has been documented also in the review by Zych, Ortega-Ruiz, and Del Rey (2015) , which showed how studies on bullying are underrepresented and undercited with regard to poorer countries and minority samples.

Looking at studies conducted on prevalence rates, it emerges that cyberbullying is a widespread problem involving a significant number of children and adolescents both as cyberbullies and cybervictims (see Table 1 for a summary of main results). However, as shown in Table 1 , prevalence rates vary, and direct comparisons are not always possible due to the different methods used and procedures.

Table 1

Prevalence Studies on Cyberbullying and Prevalence Rates.

Study  Method  N  Age range  Location  Reference period  Percentages of cyberbullying  
            CV  CB  CB/CV  NI 
Ybarra and Mitchell (2004)  Telephone survey  1,501  10-17 years  U.S.  Past 12 months  4%  12%  3%  81% 
Kowalski and Limber (2007)  Self-report survey  3,767  11-14 years  U.S.  Past couple of months  11.1%  4.1%  6.8%  78% 
Ybarra, Diener-West, and Leaf (2007)   Telephone survey  1,588  10-15 years  U.S.  Past 12 months  35%  65% 
Raskauskas and Stoltz (2007)  Self-report survey  84  13-18 years  U.S.  Within the current school year  49%  21% 
Beran and Li (2008)  Self-report survey  432  12-15 years  Canada  In participants experience  58%  26% 
Hinduja and Patchin (2008)  Online survey  1,378  < 18 years  Online recruitment  In participants experience  34.6%  16.8% 
Slonje and Smith (2008)  Self-report survey  360  12-20 years  Sweden  Past couple of months  11.7%  10.3% 
Smith et al. (2008)  Self-reported questionnaire  533  11-16 years  UK  From never to the last week or month  17.3%  12.4% 
Ortega, Elipe, Mora- Merchán, Calmaestra, and Vega (2009)   Self-report survey  1,671  12-17 years  Spain  Past 2 months  10%  90% 
Wang, Iannotti, and Nansel (2009)   Self-report survey  7,182  11-16 years  U.S.  Past couple of months  5.3%  3.8%  4.5%  86.4% 
McGuckin, Cummins, and Lewis (2010)   Self-report survey  3,699  11 years  North Ireland  In participants experience  10.3%  3.4% 
Kowalski, Morgan, and Limber (2012)   Self-report survey  4,531  11-19 years  U.S.  Past couple of months  17.3%  10.9% 
Mishna, Khoury-Kassabri, Gadalla, and Daciuk (2012)   Self-report survey  2,186  10-17 years  Canada  Past 3 months  23.8%  8%  25.7%  42.5% 
Mura and Diamantini (2013)  Self-report survey  359  14-19 years  Colombia  Past 6 months  16%  9%  53%  22% 
Kowalski and Limber (2013)  Self-report survey  931  11-19 years  U.S.  Past couple of months  9.9%  6.1%  5.3%  78.7% 
Sticca, Ruggieri, Alsaker, and Perren (2013)   Self-report survey  835  Mean age =13.2 years  Swiss  Past 4 months  22%  14% 
Van Cleemput, Vandebosch, and Pabian (2014)   Self-report survey  2,333  9-16 years  Belgium  Past 6 months  11.1%  11.1%  3.8% 
Yang et al. (2014)  School survey  1,173  13 years  Korea  Not mentioned  19.2% 
Wong, Chan, and Cheng (2014)   Self-report survey  1,917  12-15 years  China  Past month  23%  31.5% 
Callaghan, Kelly, and Molcho (2015)   Self-report survey  318  15-18 years  Ireland  Past couple of months  9.8%  66.3% 
Tarablus, Heiman, and Olenik-Shemesh (2015)   Self-report survey  458  11-13 years  Israel  Past couple of months  8.9%  5.4%  85.7% 
Waasdorp and Bradshaw (2015)  Self-report survey  28,104  14-18 years  U.S.  Past month  4.6%  77.3% 

Note . Studies not reporting the cyberbully/cybervictim category (-) means that in those studies authors reported the percentages of cyberbullying (yes/no) and cybervictimisation (yes/no) without looking at the overlap group.

What emerges from this review is that whereas studies on cyberbullying report prevalence rates for involvement in cyberbullying and cybervictimisation, few report the overlap category of cyberbully and cybervictim. We will here focus on this overlap category to identify gaps in the literature and therefore conduct a study to address them so to provide outcomes of use for dedicated intervention programs. What is worth mentioning in a first look at all studies reported in Table 1 is that prevalence rates vary from 4% up to 34% indicating a variance not so much in absolute incidence or prevalence rates, but in inconsistency of methodologies, definitions, sampling, and methods.

An early and extensive study carried out by Ybarra and Mitchell (2004) between 1999 and 2000 showed that 19% of internet users ( N = 1,501) were involved in cyberbullying either as cyberbullies, cybervictims, or both. Wang, Iannotti, and Nansel (2009) collected data from 7,182 American students during 2005 and 2006. The study highlighted that of students involved in cyberbullying 32.6% were both cyberbullies and cybervictims. Kowalski and Limber (2007) found that 7% had been involved in cyberbullying both as a bully and a victim. In the same year, Raskauskas and Stoltz (2007) found higher cyberbullying prevalence rates. In fact 49% of the students that they surveyed ( N = 84) reported that they were cybervictims and 21% stated that they were cyberbullies. The sample, however, was rather small and not representative.

Slonje and Smith (2008) surveyed 360 Swedish adolescents to investigate the extent and nature of cyberbullying. The results highlighted that 11.7% of the whole sample reported being a victim of cyberbullying and 10.3% reported being a cyberbully.

Smith et al. (2008) surveyed 533 secondary school students aged 11-16 years in England to investigate the relationship between school and cyberbullying. The authors found a substantial continuity of the roles of bullies and victims. Cybervictims were more often also involved as school victims, while cyberbullies were also school bullies.

Also, Beran and Li (2008) , in Canada, found that 58% of the students they surveyed had experienced cybervictimisation while 26% were cyberbullies in their life course. In the same year, Hinduja and Patchin (2008), in line with Ybarra, Diener-West, and Leaf (2007) , found that about 35% of the adolescents participating in their research had experienced at least one cyberbullying incident as a victim.

Ortega, Ellipe, Mora-Merchán, Calmaestra, and Vega (2009) , in Spain, reported that 25% of participants were victims of some kind of bullying, 5% were cybervictims only, and 5% reported “multivictimisation” (they were both traditional victims and cybervictims).

McGuckin, Cummins, and Lewis (2010) surveyed 3,699 primary school students in Northern Ireland about their life experiences of school bullying and cyberbullying. Data were collected between 2008 and 2009 and showed that about one student in ten (10.3%) was a victim of cyberbullying and 3.4% of all respondents reported they had cyberbullied others.

Mishna, Khoury-Kassabri, Gadalla, and Daciuk (2012) in Canada examined the frequency of cyberbullying in the previous three months, among 2,186 middle and high school students. Looking at the overlap category, one in four students (25.7%) reported overlapping categories. Kowalski and Limber (2013) with their US sample had a 5.3% cyberbullies/cybervictims overlap.

Kowalski, Morgan, and Limber (2012) investigated the relationship between students’ involvement in school bullying and cyberbullying, by surveying 4,531 US youth. The results showed that 37.8% of participants were school victims and 17.3% were cybervictims, while 31.8% were school bullies and 10.9% were cyberbullies.

Mura and Diamantini (2013) analysed the cyberbullying prevalence in Colombia by surveying 359 adolescents. Their aim was to investigate if youth in developing countries such as Colombia are exposed to similar problems as adolescents living in industrialised countries. The results showed that over 2/3 of students reported being involved in cyberbullying, 16% were cybervictims, and 9% cyberbullies. In another completely different geographical area, in Switzerland, Sticca, Ruggieri, Alsaker, and Perren (2013) found that 14% of respondents were involved in cyberbullying as bullies and 22% reported some form of cybervictimisation in the past four months.

Van Cleemput, Vandebosch, and Pabian (2014) found that out of the 2,333 Flemish students they surveyed, 11.1% were involved in cyberbullying as a perpetrator, and the same percentage reported to be a victim of cyberbullying, during the previous 6 month period.

Yang et al. (2014) carried out a two-year prospective school survey and found that 19.2% of the 1,173 Korean students participating in the study were cybervictims, though these authors do not indicate the time reference period they used. In Hong Kong, China, Wong, Chan, and Cheng (2014) surveyed 1,917 secondary students to explore the prevalence of cyberbullying. The results suggested that about a third of participants (31.5%) reported being involved in cyberbullying perpetration and 23% reported being victimised by someone in the cyberworld in a period of time of one month.

Callaghan, Kelly, and Molcho (2015) surveyed 318 Irish students about their experience of school bullying and cyberbullying and found that 14.3% and 9.8% were respectively traditional victims and cybervictims, while 9.5% were involved in both school and cybervictimisation.

In Israel, Tarablus, Heiman, and Olenik-Shemesh (2015) surveyed 458 junior high school students to investigate the overlap between school bullying and cyberbullying. Results highlighted that 22.2% of students were both school and cybervictims, and that 25% of them were involved both as school bullies and cyberbullies but they did not look at the overlap between cyberbullying and cybervictimisation. This was often found in studies, no classification of cyberbullying into the four categories, as shown in Table 1 , so that it is not always possible to report the overlap of the two roles.

Waasdorp and Bradshaw (2015) carried out a study involving 28,104 US adolescents and found that 4.6% were cybervictims-only, while of those involved, 50.3% reported experiencing all the four forms of victimisation considered (verbal, physical, relational, and cybervictimisation).

What these studies is that the prevalence of cyberbullying reported varies not only with the age group and country, but mainly it could be due to differences in the methodology and measures used. Not all studies are consistent in classifying students as only cyberbullies, only cybervictims, or both (or not at all involved).

The aims of our study were:

  • -

    To investigate the prevalence rates of cyberbullying and cybervictimisation from different areas in the country.

  • -

    To investigate gender differences with regard to different levels of involvement in cyberbullying and cybervictimisation.

  • -

    To investigate the overlap between cyberbullying and school bullying.

  • -

    To investigate the overlap between cybervictimisation and school victimisation.

Method Participants

The total sample consisted of 5,058 Italian students, 47% boys and 53% girls. This is the largest study on cyberbullying published on Italian 10-18 year old students. The mean age was 15.6 years ( SD = 2.87) and 97.2% were Italian and the rest were born abroad. Almost all participants (99%) reported using the internet and 89.3% had at least a social network profile and on average spent 3 hours online (see Table 2).

Table 2

Descriptive Statistics of the Initial Sample ( N = 5,058).

Measures and items   
Age  Mean = 15.6, SD = 2.87 
Gender  53% females, 47% males 
Nationality  97.2% Italian, 2.8% non EU immis 
internet at home  99% have internet at home 
Number of hours a day students use the web  Mean = 2.87, SD = 2.70 
Use of mobile phone while at school  39.3% ‘yes’, 60.7% ‘no’ 
Social network profile  89.3% ‘yes, at least one’, 10.7% ‘none’ 

Participants were recruited from a total of nine different schools that took part in a larger project named Openeyes aiming at addressing and setting up strategies to prevent and reduce cyberbullying.

Two middle schools (11-14 year-old students) and seven high schools (14-18 year-old students) located in two different Italian sites, Milan and surrounding provinces (Northern Italy) and Naples and surrounding provinces (Southern Italy), took part in the study. One middle and one high school were private schools. So, only two schools among those initially contacted declined to take part in the study due to lack of time. These were d by two others which were comparable with respect to socio-economic conditions.

The schools were representative of the types of schools for students aged 10-18 in Italy (middle and high schools) and represented a variety of socio-economic statuses, although schools were not randomly ed. Sampling of participants was at the school level, which implied correcting results for clustered data. Therefore, we calculated intraclass correlations (ICCs) and reported effect size ( d ) and their adjusted significance.

Most schools in Italy are public but there is a significant proportion of private (mostly Catholic) schools. Throughout all analyses we checked for significant differences between the private and public schools, but none emerged so the whole sample was merged together. All classes of the participating schools were d in the study, with the exception of the top class of high schools. For the description of the sample and other details, see Table 2.

Procedure

Parental consent was obtained before the start of the data collection, and a contact email address and a phone number of the main researcher was made available for teachers and parents who wished to have further information about the study and the procedure. The whole content of the questionnaire was not disclosed to students nor to parents to avoid response contamination. After obtaining parental consent, students were approached in the classrooms according to a schedule organized by the head of the school. They were told the purpose of the study and the procedure, and were told that if they wanted they could withdraw and be involved in some other activities while their peers filled in the anonymous questionnaire for approximately 30 minutes. No one contacted the researcher for further explanations and no student withdrew from the study.

The self-reported paper and pencil questionnaires were administered to students in their classes. The students were seated at a distance from each other to prevent interference or talking. Participants were told that it was not an exam so there were no wrong or right answers but that they had to refer to their experience with peers at school and online. They were told that they did not have to put their names on the forms so as to guarantee their anonymity. Before starting to fill in the questionnaire, the terms school bullying and cyberbullying were briefly explained to make sure that the students understood what we were referring to. The items used to measure school bullying and cyberbullying, though never directly used the term bullying but described each behaviour considered to be bullying (or cyberbullying). While completing the questionnaire, one of the research assistants was present to provide explanations if needed and to oversee the procedure. Data collection took place between October 2011 and December 2011.

Measures

The questionnaire consisted of several scales measuring the dimensions under investigation. For the purpose of the present paper two different scales were analysed: to measure school bullying we used the sum of 7 items measuring different types of direct and indirect bullying, that might have taken place in the previous six months (e.g., ‘I called names’, ‘I threatened someone at school’, ‘I excluded someone on purpose’; ? = .79). For each item respondents could answer on a 5-point scale (0-4), the categories being it never happened, it happened once or twice, sometimes, once a week, and several times a week . The final measure used was a dichotomous one d by classifying as ‘not bullies’ those who rated either never or once or twice in all of the 7 types of behaviours. Bullies were those who said that they did any of the listed behaviours at least sometimes in the previous six months. The same criteria were used for victimisation. The method of using the cut-off of ‘at least sometimes’ was preferred so as to exclude those who admitted any of the behaviours listed only once or twice, which is not considered bullying due to lack of repetition in time ( Farrington, 1993).

To measure cyberbullying we made use of the scale developed by Willard (2007) . For the purpose of the present paper, we used the following 5 items measuring different types of cyberbullying: ‘I sent mean, cruel, or threatening messages online to someone I knew’, ‘I humiliated someone online by sending or posting offensive material or cruel messages’, ‘I pretended to be someone else, d a fake profile in order to send or post damaging messages about another person’, ‘I disclosed online private information or images without the person consent’, and ‘I was actively engaged in excluding someone from an online group’ (? = .71). Respondents had to indicate for each of the cyberbehaviours whether it ‘never happened’, whether ‘it happened 1 to 4 times’, or ‘5 or more times’ in the previous six months. To use the same criteria as the one used for school bullying and based on the definition and classification of cyberbullying ( Patchin & Hinduja, 2006 ), we used the most conservative definition corresponding to at least sometimes. Each student was classified as a cyberbully if he or she had committed any of the behaviours listed 5 or more times in the previous six months; if they never did anything or did it from 1 to 4 times, they were classified as a non-cyberbully . The same criteria was used to measure cybervictimisation. These criteria are questionable due to the debate of whether, in defining online harassment as actual cyberbullying, you need repetition or not ( Patchin & Hinduja, 2015 ). For the purpose of the current study we wanted to be conservative and use the most comparable measures of school bullying and cyberbullying.

In the questionnaire, we also asked some other descriptive questions about students’ use of the web and how many hours they spent online, and whether they used their mobile phones when at school to communicate with others (details are provided in Table 2 ). These questions are not further analysed in this article.

Results

In order to investigate gender differences in involvement in cyberbullying, we compared means as shown in Table 3. We d values because these measure the strength of the relationships; F and t tests are greatly affected by sample size as well as by the strength of the relationship and so they are not pure measures of association. The variance of d was corrected for clustering using the following equation (see Hedges & Hedberg, 2007):

Table 3

Gender Differences in Individual Types of Cyberbullying and Cybervictimisation.

  N of students  Mean score d  p  ICC 
    Girls  Boys       
Cybervictimisation
Overall cybervictimisation  4991  .54  .53  0.01  ns  .021 
Specific types of cybervictimisation
I’ve received online messages that made me very afraid for my safety’  4966  .10  .08  0.05  ns  .002 
I’ve been put down online by someone who has sent or posted cruel gossip, rumours, or other harmful material’  4967  .17  .14  0.08  ns  .016 
‘Someone pretended to be me and sent or posted you material that damaged my reputation or friendships’  4969  .12  .14  0.06  ns  .018 
‘Someone shared my personal secrets or images online without my permission’  4996  .11  .11  0.01  ns  .001 
‘I’ve been excluded on spite from an online group by people who have been mean to me’  4970  .04  .05  0.05  ns  .007 
Cyberbullying
Overall cyberbullying  4991  .39  .81  0.34  .016  .041 
Specific types of cyberbullying
‘I sent mean, cruel or threatening messages online to someone I knew’  4955  .11  .28  0.36  .005  .035 
‘Put down someone online by sending cruel gossip, rumors, or other harmful material’  4970  .09  .18  0.21  .041  .022 
‘Pretend to be someone else and send material to damage that person's reputation or friendships’  4975  .07  .12  0.15  ns  .018 
‘Share someone's personal secrets or images online without his or her consent’  4966  .05  .10  0.16  .007  .006 
‘Help to exclude a peer or student from an online group’  4969  .07  .14  0.22  .026  .020 

Note . With regard to cyberbullying and cybervictimisation values are measured on a 3-point scale from 0 to 2. Differences in N numbers are due to missing values. Effect sizes have been corrected for clustering. Adjusted significance values are presented, two tailed.

** p < .01, *** p < .001

Vcorr = V [1 + (n - 1) * ICC]

where,

Vcorr = corrected variance

V = usual variance of d

n = average cluster size (school size)

ICC = intraclass correlation

The intraclass correlations are shown on the right hand columns of Table 3.

Boys were more involved than girls in overall cyberbullying and in particular forms of cyberbullying such as sending mean, cruel, or threatening messages online to someone they know, whereas no significant gender differences were found with regard to cybervictimisation (see Table 3 for details and significance).

We then used the categorical classification for the four possible levels of involvement in school bullying and then for cyberbullying to look for gender differences. We used the odd ratios ( OR ) to measure strengths of relationships because (unlike chi-squared for example) they are not influenced by sample size. We corrected the variance of the ORs for clustering as exampled above 1 1

Various dichotomies were d for school bullying: only school bullying versus the rest, only school victimisation vs. the rest, school bully/victim vs. the rest. For cyberbullying: only cyberbullying vs. the rest, only cybervictimisation vs. the rest, cyber bully/victim vs. the rest. This enables the calculation of OR s in 2 x 2 for gender (boys vs. girls).

.

With regard to school bullying, the odds of only bullying were 1.71 greater for boys than for girls. Comparing the percentages (16.6 vs. 10.4), boys were 1.60 times more likely to be bullies. The ICC for only school bullying was .005, and the corrected CI was 1.06-3.26 ( p < .001, two tailed).

The odds for only victims were 1.35 greater for girls than for boys. Comparing the percentages (13.9 vs. 10.7), girls were 1.30 times more likely to be school victims. The ICC for only school victims was .007, and the corrected CI was .91-2.00 ( ns).

The odds for school bully/victim were 1.59 greater for boys than girls. Comparing the percentages (31.6 vs. 22.5), boys were 1.44 times more likely to be school bully/victims. The ICC for school bully/victims was .036, the corrected intervals are .88-2.86 ( ns), see (Table 4).

Table 4

Gender Differences in School Bullying.

  Not involved  Only bully  Bully/Victim  Only victim  Total 
Gender
Male
933  376  716  242  2267 
Row%  41.2  16.6  31.6  10.7  100 
Col%  40.5  58.3  55.2  40.3  46.7 
Total  19.2  7.8  14.8  5.0  46.7 
Female
1373  269  582  359  2583 
Row%  53.2  10.4  22.5  13.9  100 
Col%  59.5  41.7  44.8  59.7  53.3 
Total  28.3  5.5  12.0  7.4  53.3 
Total
2306  645  1298  601  4850 
Row%  47.5  13.3  26.8  12.4  100 
Col%  100  100  100  100  100 
Total  47.5  13.3  26.8  12.4  100 

Note . Values indicate row percentages first and then column percentages. Percentages refer to bullying other at least sometimes in the previous six months.

OR for OB vs. rest = 1.71, adjusted CI = 1.06 - 3.26 (p < .001).

OR for OV vs. rest = 1.35, adjusted CI = .91 - 2.00 (ns)

OR for BV vs. rest = 1.59, adjusted CI = .88 - 2.86 (ns)

OR = odds ratio.

OB = only school bully, OV = only school victim, BV = school bully/victims.

With regard to cyberbullying, again, the odds ratio for only cyberbullies were 3.55 greater for boys than girls. Comparing the percentages (7.1 vs. 2.1), boys were 3.38 times more likely to be only cyberbullies. The ICC for only cyberbullying was .017 and the corrected intervals were 1.30-9.77 ( p < .001, two tailed).

Only cybervictims were not significantly different between boys and girls ( OR = 1.09, corrected CI = .75 to 1.57 (p = ns).

The odds for cyberbully/victims were 3.59 greater for boys than girls. Comparing the percentages (3.1 vs. 0.9), boys were 3.4 times more likely to be cyberbully/victims. The ICC for cyberbully/victims was .011, the corrected CI 1.03-12.55 ( p < .05, two tailed) (see Table 5).

Table 5

Gender Differences in Cyberbullying.

  Not involved  Only cyberbully  Cyberbully/cybervictim  Only cybervictim  Total 
Gender
Male
1954  158  70  52  2234 
Row%  87.5  7.1  3.1  2.3  100 
Col%  44.6  74.5  75.3  44.4  46.5 
Total  40.6  3.3  1.5  1.1  46.5 
Female
2432  54  23  65  2574 
Row%  94.5  2.1  .9  2.5  100 
Col%  55.4  25.5  24.7  55.6  53.5 
Total  50.6  1.1  .5  1.4  53.5 
Total
4386  212  93  117  4808 
Row%  91.2  4.4  1.9  2.4  100 
Col%  100  100  100  100  100 
Total  91.2  4.4  1.9  2.4  100 

Note . Values indicate row percentages first and then column percentages. Percentages refer to cyberbullying five times or more in the previous six months.

OR for OCB vs. rest = 3.55, adjusted CI = 1.30 - 9.77 (p < .001).

OR for OCV vs. rest = 1.09, adjusted CI = 75 - 1.57( ns)

OR for CBV vs. rest = 3.59, adjusted CI = 1.03 - 12.55 (p < .05)

OR = odd ratio.

OCB = only cyberbully, OCV = only cybervictim, CBV = cyberbully/victim.

The Overlap between School Bullying and Cyberbullying

We then wanted to investigate the relationship between school bullying and cyberbullying, to know whether students who bully peers at school also do it online. Of all students bullying others at least sometimes at school, 12.1% were also cyberbullies at least sometimes, compared to only 2.6% of those who did not bully at school. Bullies were 4.6 times more likely to be cyberbullies according to the odds ratio, as reported in Table 6 . The ICC for school bullying was .032, and the ICC for cyberbullying was .042; therefore we used the average of .037 to correct for the clustering in schools. The corrected CI was 2.40-10.42 ( p < .0001, two tailed).

Table 6

Overlap between School Bullying and Cyberbullying ( N = 4,817).

    School bullying  
Cyberbullying    NO  YES  Total 
  NO
Row %
Col % 
2813
(62.4)
(97.4) 
1697
(37.6)
(87.9) 
4510
(100)
(93.6) 
  YES
Row %
Col % 
74
(24.1)
(2.6) 
233
(75.9)
(12.1) 
307
(100)
(6.4) 
Total 
Row %
Col % 
2887
(59.9)
(100) 
1930
(40.1)
(100) 
4817
(100)
(100) 

Note . Values in parentheses indicate row percentages first and then column percentages. Percentages refer to bullying others at least sometimes and cyberbullying five times or more in the previous six months.

OR = 4.6, adjusted CI = 2.40 - 10.42, p < .0001, two tailed.

The overlap between school victimisation and cybervictimisation

Of all students victimised at least sometimes at school, 7.4% were also cybervictims, compared to 2.2% of those were not victimised at school. Victims were 3.23 times more likely to be cybervictims according to the odds ratio, as reported in Table 7 . The ICC for school victimisation was .043, and the ICC for cybervictimisation was .019, so we used the average of .031 to correct for the clustering in schools. The corrected CI was 1.79-5.84 ( p < .0001, two tailed).

Table 7

Overlap between School Victimisation and Cybervictimisation ( N = 4,779).

    Victimisation  
Cybervictimisation    NO  YES  Total 
  NO
Row %
Col % 
2846
(62.2)
(97.8) 
1729
(37.8)
(92.6) 
4575
(100)
(95.7) 
  YES
Row %
Col % 
65
(31.9)
(2.2) 
139
(68.1)
(7.4) 
204
(100)
(4.3) 
Total 
Row %
Col % 
2911
(60.9)
(100) 
1868
(39.1)
(100) 
4779
(100)
(100) 

Note . Values in parentheses indicate row percentages first and then column percentages. Percentages refer to victimisation others at least sometimes and cybervictimisation five times or more in the previous six months.

OR = 3.23, adjusted CI = 1.79 - 5.84, p < .0001, two tailed.

Discussion and Conclusions

Cyberbullying is a serious problem affecting a considerable number of children from primary to secondary school and it can go on until the high school years. In our study we found high prevalence rates and a significant overlap between students admitting bullying at school as well as bullying online and between being a cyberbully and a cybervictim. As soon as a child starts using the internet and other cyberdevices to communicate and interact, he or she is potentially at risk of becoming a target of cyberbullying or else of cyberbullying. These findings provide grounds for follow up hypotheses. On one hand, it is important to look at the relationship between these two behaviours to understand whether there is a ‘continuity’ of school bullying and cyberbullying; students who are school bullies will go on outside the school to do similar things to the same or different victims when at home ( Raskauskas & Stoltz, 2007 ). On the other hand, our findings are also of use for a possible alternative interpretation: whether school victims will go online and start (cyber)bullying others, and invert their role ( Ybarra & Mitchell, 2004).

Having drawn our sample from several schools, we corrected for clustering. The results indicated that, with regard to cybervictimisation, boys and girls were equally likely to be involved, whereas with regard to cyberbullying boys were up to 3 times more likely to act as either as an only cyberbully or as a cyberbully/cybervictim.

The strength of the relationship between gender and involvement in the four different categories of school bullying was less strong than in cyberbullying, indicating that school bullying still deserves attention and investigation, especially with regard to more and more girls involved in these behaviours. Age differences could explain part of these results, meaning that school bullying is an age related issue, rather than a gender difference, with more older girls involved in school bullying. This field of research is not fully addressed and explained, since it is assumed that bullying decreases with age. Lack of information about web risks, poor parental or teacher supervision, isolation, low empathy, and moral disengagement might all be factors contributing to the rise of this phenomenon.

In our study we used a rather conservative criterion to define a student as a cyberbully (or cybervictim): whether it happened at least 5 times in the last 6 months, and not just once or twice, following the criteria first used by Ortega, Calmaestra, and Mora-Merchán (2008) . This criterion led to a slightly lower prevalence rate than the one found in previous studies ( Smith et al., 2008 ). This might be a limitation, and in future studies more detailed questions about frequency could be used. Also, a better relationship between frequency and intensity and prevalence should be measured.

One could argue that one cyberaction can potentially produce the same devastating effect as repeated actions due to how the internet works. However, we wanted to use a similar definition to the one used for school bullying to identify students who have been exposed to or committed these behaviours repeatedly.

The current study is useful also for its implications, since it stresses once more that there is an overlap between school bullying and cyberbullying, so intervention campaigns should always address both antisocial behaviours since the risk factors for them are similar ( Baldry et al., 2015 ). Looking at the school bully/victim and cyberbully/cybervictim categories, it is interesting to note that the overlap does not imply that the students who are bullied (or cyberbullied) will eventually bully or cyberbully themselves. To understand the direction of such relationships, longitudinal studies are needed; however, we know by now that the two are related.

Limitations of the studies are related to the measures used for school and cyberbullying that were differently measured and comparisons should be made with caution. Also, given that this is a correlational study and not a longitudinal one, it is not possible to draw causal conclusions about school bullying and cyberbullying.

To conclude, and based also on the difficulty of comparing studies, as Patchin and Hinduja (2015) correctly pointed out, we would like to draw attention to the following critical points when gathering data on the prevalence of cyberbullying and provide some recommendations for researchers as well as practitioners:

Age groups: all studies should specify the age range they used and report data according to age range and not school grade since in different countries different grade systems are used.

Gender differences should always be taken into account.

In investigating prevalence, cyberbullying and cybervictimisation should be measured not by directly asking students if they are a victim (or perpetrator) of cyberbullying, because the likelihood of having socially desirable answers and different understandings of the meaning of the words is high. Therefore, separate type of behaviours should be asked about separately.

When measuring the frequency of episodes, the exact reference period should be specified (not asking ‘in the last term’): in the last month, or 3 or 6 months, or ever depending on the aims of the study.

If the number of episodes are measured (1, 2-4 or more), then frequency and recency should be compared. If a pupil received intimidating messages in the last few days, 2 or 3 episodes might constitute bullying. In a 6-month period or even ever, 2 or 3 episodes could be less troubling. It is better to measure prevalence rates weekly or monthly (e.g., once or twice a week, once or twice a month).

If both school bullying and cyberbullying are measured and researchers want to draw comparisons between the two forms of bullying (online and not), the same frequency and time frame should be used for each of the two behaviours (school and cyber).

Conflict of Interest

The authors of this article declare no conflict of interest.

References
Baldry et al., 2015
A.C. Baldry
D. Farrington
A. Sorrentino
Am I at risk of cyberbullying”?. A narrative review and conceptual framework for research on risk of cyberbullying and cybervictimization: The risk and needs assessment approach
Aggression and Violent Behavior
23
2015
36-51
Beran and Li, 2008
T. Beran
Q. Li
The relationship between cyberbullying and school bullying
Journal of Student Wellbeing
1
2
2008
16-33
Callaghan et al., 2015
M. Callaghan
C. Kelly
M. Molcho
Exploring traditional and cyberbullying among Irish adolescents
International Journal of Public Health
60
2
2015
199-206
Del Rey et al., 2015
R. Del Rey
J.A. Casas
R. Ortega-Ruiz
A. Schultze-Krumbholz
H. Scheithauer
P. Smith
Structural validation and cross-cultural robustness of the European Cyberbullying Intervention Project Questionnaire
Computers in Human Behavior
50
2015
141-7
Farrington, 1993
D.P. Farrington
Understanding and preventing bullying
University of Chicago Press
M.TonryN.Morris
Chicago
1993
381-458
Hedges and Hedberg, 2007
L.V. Hedges
E.C. Hedberg
Interclass correlations for planning group randomized experiments in rural education
Journal of Research in Rural Education
22
10
2007
1-15
Hinduja and Patchin, 2008
S. Hinduja
J.W. Patchin
Cyberbullying: An exploratory analysis of factors related to offending and victimization
Deviant Behavior
29
2
2008
129-56
Kowalski et al., 2014
R.M. Kowalski
G.W. Giumetti
A.N. Schroeder
M.R. Lattanner
Bullying in the digital age: A critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth
Psychological Bulletin
140
4
2014
1073-137
Kowalski and Limber, 2007
R.M. Kowalski
S.P. Limber
Electronic bullying among middle school students
Journal of Adolescent Health
41
6
2007
S22-30
Kowalski and Limber, 2013
R.M. Kowalski
S.P. Limber
Psychological, physical, and academic correlates of cyberbullying and traditional bullying
Journal of Adolescent Health
53
1
2013
S13-20
Kowalski et al., 2012
R.M. Kowalski
C.A. Morgan
S.P. Limber
Traditional bullying as a potential warning sign of cyberbullying
School Psychology International
33
5
2012
505-19
McGuckin et al., 2010
C. McGuckin
P.K. Cummins
C.A. Lewis
f2f and cyberbullying among children in Northern Ireland: Data from the Kids Life and Times Surveys
Psychology, Society & Education
2
2
2010
83-96
Mishna et al., 2012
F. Mishna
M. Khoury-Kassabri
T. Gadalla
J. Daciuk
Risk factors for involvement in cyber bullying: Victims, bullies and bully-victims
Children and Youth Services Review
34
1
2012
63-70
Mura and Diamantini, 2013
G. Mura
D. Diamantini
Cyberbullying among Colombian students: An exploratory investigation
European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education
3
3
2013
249-56
Olweus, 2012
D. Olweus
Cyberbullying: An overrated phenomenon?
European Journal of Developmental Psychology
9
5
2012
520-38
Olweus, 2013
D. Olweus
School bullying: Development and some important challenges
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology
9
2013
751-80
Ortega et al., 2008
R. Ortega
J. Calmaestra
J.A. Mora-Merchán
Cyberbullying
International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy
8
2
2008
183-92
Ortega et al., 2009
R. Ortega
P. Elipe
J.A. Mora-Merchán
J. Calmaestra
E. Vega
The emotional impact on victims of traditional bullying and cyberbullying. A Study of Spanish Adolescents
Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology
217
4
2009
197-204
Patchin and Hinduja, 2006
J.W. Patchin
S. Hinduja
Bullies move beyond the schoolyard a preliminary look at cyberbullying
Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice
4
2
2006
148-69
Patchin and Hinduja, 2015
J.W. Patchin
S. Hinduja
Measuring Cyberbullying: Implications for Research
Aggression and Violent Behavior
23
2015
69-74
Raskauskas and Stoltz, 2007
J. Raskauskas
A.D. Stoltz
Involvement in traditional and electronic bullying among adolescents
Developmental Psychology
43
3
2007
564-75
Slonje and Smith, 2008
R. Slonje
P.K. Smith
Cyberbullying: Another main type of bullying?
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology
49
2
2008
147-54
Slonje et al., 2013
R. Slonje
P.K. Smith
A. Frisén
The nature of cyberbullying, and strategies for prevention
Computers in Human Behavior
29
2013
26-32
Smith et al., 2013
P.K. Smith
C. del Barrio
R.S. Tokunaga
Definitions of bullying and cyberbullying: How useful are the terms?
Principles of cyberbullying research: Definitions measures and methodology
Routledge
New York
2013
26-45
Smith et al., 2008
P.K. Smith
J. Mahdavi
M. Carvalho
S. Fisher
S. Russell
N. Tippett
Cyberbullying: Its nature and impact in secondary school pupils
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
49
4
2008
376-85
Sticca et al., 2013
F. Sticca
S. Ruggieri
F. Alsaker
S. Perren
Longitudinal risk factors for cyberbullying in adolescence
Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology
23
2013
52-67
Tarablus et al., 2015
T. Tarablus
T. Heiman
D. Olenik-Shemesh
Cyber bullying among teenagers in Israel: An examination of cyberbullying, traditional bullying, and socioemotional Functioning
Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma
24
6
2015
707-20
Tokunaga, 2010
R.S. Tokunaga
Following you home from school: A critical review and synthesis of research on cyberbullying victimization
Computers in Human Behavior
26
3
2010
277-87
Van Cleemput et al., 2014
K. Van Cleemput
H. Vandebosch
S. Pabian
Personal characteristics and contextual factors that determine “helping,”“ing in” and “doing nothing” when witnessing cyberbullying
Aggressive Behavior
40
5
2014
383-96
Waasdorp and Bradshaw, 2015
T.E. Waasdorp
C.P. Bradshaw
The overlap between cyberbullying and traditional bullying
Journal of Adolescent Health
56
5
2015
483-8
Wang et al., 2009
J. Wang
R.J. Iannotti
T.R. Nansel
School bullying among adolescents in the United States: Physical, verbal, relational, and cyber
Journal of Adolescent Health
45
4
2009
368-75
Willard, 2007
N.E. Willard
Cyberbullying and cyberthreats: Responding to the challenge of online social aggression, threats, and distress
2007
Wolak et al., 2007
J. Wolak
K.J. Mitchell
D. Finkelhor
Does online harassment constitute bullying?. An exploration of online harassment by known peers and online-only contacts
Journal of Adolescent Health
41
6
2007
S51-8
Wong et al., 2014
D.S. Wong
H.C.O. Chan
C.H. Cheng
Cyberbullying perpetration and victimization among adolescents in Hong Kong
Children and Youth Services Review
36
2014
133-40
Yang et al., 2014
S.J. Yang
R. Stewart
J.Y. Lee
J.M. Kim
S.W. Kim
I.S. Shin
J.S. Yoon
Prevalence and correlates of problematic internet experiences and computer-Using Time: A two-year longitudinal study in Korean school children
Psychiatry Investigation
11
2014
24-31
Ybarra and Mitchell, 2004
M.L. Ybarra
K.J. Mitchell
Online aggressor/targets, aggressors, and targets: A comparison of associated youth characteristics
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
45
7
2004
1308-16
Ybarra et al., 2012
M.L. Ybarra
D. Boyd
J.D. Korchmaros
J.K. Oppenheim
Defining and measuring cyberbullying within the larger context of bullying victimization
Journal of Adolescent Health
51
2012
53-8
Ybarra et al., 2007
M.L. Ybarra
M. Diener-West
P.J. Leaf
Examining the overlap in internet harassment and school bullying: Implications for school intervention
Journal of Adolescent Health
41
6
2007
S42-50
Zych et al., 2015
I. Zych
R. Ortega-Ruiz
R. Del Rey
Scientific research on bullying and cyberbullying: where have we been and where are we going
Aggression and Violent Behavior
24
2015
188-98

Copyright © 2024. Colegio Oficial de la Psicología de Madrid

© Copyright 2024. Colegio Oficial de la Psicología de Madrid ContactoPolítica de privacidadPolítica de cookies

Utilizamos cookies propias y de terceros para mejorar nuestros servicios y conocer sus preferencias mediante el análisis de sus hábitos de navegación. Si continua navegando, consideramos que acepta su uso. Puede acceder a política de cookies para obtener más información.

Aceptar